Sources of information we use when reasoning about the causes of our behaviour

Aleš Neusar

Abstract

We do not commonly think about the causes of our behaviour in everyday life except when we have a relevant reason. Our interest in causes of our behaviour may be raised by researcher’s (or a therapist’s) question or by the situation when the motive of our action is critical, for example, for feelings of achievement or for evaluating our behaviour as moral. Attributed causes of our behaviour may not be the same as its real causes. We never know all the influences upon our behaviour. The process of reasoning about causes is an interpretative one and is affected not only by accessible information but also by the type of question asked, our education, defence mechanisms, our way of reasoning, etc. We often reason about these causes retrospectively and our reasoning is thus affected by memory deficiencies and by the way we choose which memories to deal with. Reasoning about causes is usually not a time-consuming rational process, when we consider all available information. We typically employ quicker and more effective heuristics, which is optimal because of the lack of time and cognitive processing resources available. Some information about the causes of our action is accessible to us only through introspection. Other information is possessed by other people, e.g. shared causal theories. We tend to overestimate introspective information because of its “authenticity”. Even though we may not know the crucial influences on our behaviour, this may not prevent us from being able to report on the causes and from being convinced that we know the actual causes. So it seems that it is not fundamental whether we become aware of the causal stimulus or not, but whether we believe that this stimulus influenced us.

(Fulltext in Czech)

Keywords

cause of behaviour, causal reasoning, consciousness, controlled and automatic behaviour

Literature

Augoustinos, M., Walker, I. & Donaghue, N. (2006). Social cognition. An integrated introduction (2nd ed.). London: Sage.

Baars, B. J. (2003). How brain reveals mind: neural studies support the fundamental role of conscious experience. In A. Jack & A. Roepstorff (Eds.), Trusting the subject? The use of introspective evidence in cognitive science. Vol. 1 (100–114). Thorverton: Imprint Academic.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York: Freeman.

Bargh, J. A., Chen, M. & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 2, 230–244.

Bargh, J. A. (2005). Bypassing the will: toward demystifying the nonconscious control of social behavior. In R. R. Hassin, J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious (37–58). New York: Oxford University Press.

Bargh, J. A. (2007). Social psychological approaches to consciousness. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of consciousness (553–567). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Baron, J. (2000). Thinking and deciding (3rd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego-depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.

Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of social information processing goals: nonconscious priming reproduces effects of explicit conscious instructions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 464–478.

Crocker, J. (1981). Judgment of covariation by social perceivers. Psychological Bulletin, 90 (2), 272–292.

Dennet, D. M. (2008). Some observations on the psychology of thinking about free will. In J. Baer, J. C. Kaufman & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Are we free. Psychology and free will (248–259). New York: Oxford University Press.

Dutton, D. G., & Aron, A. P. (1974). Some evidence for heightened sexual attraction under conditions of high anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30 (4), 510–517.

Dweck, C. S. & Molden, D. C. (2008). Self-theories: the construction of free will. In J. Baer, J. C. Kaufman & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Are we free. Psychology and free will (44–64). New York: Oxford University Press.

Ericsson, K. A. & Simon, H. A. (1993). Protocol analysis. Verbal reports as data (rev. ed.). Cambridge: A Bradford Book. The MIT Press.

Ericsson, K. A. (2003). Valid and non-reactive verbalization of thoughts during performance of tasks: Toward a solution to the central problems of introspection as a source of scientific data. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10 (9–10), 1–18.

Gazzaniga, M. & LeDoux, J. (1978). Integrated Mind. New York: Plenum. Gigerenzer, G., Czerlinski, J. & Martignon, L. (2002). How good are fast and frugal heuristics? In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin and D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment (559–581). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Gilbert, D. T. (1989). Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference process. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought, (189–211). New York: Guilford. 

Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38.

Haney, C., Banks, C., & Zimbardo, P., (1973). Interpersonal dynamics in a simulated prison. International Journal of Criminology & Penology, 1(1), 69–97.

Heřmanská, L. (2009). Bystander effect ve virtuálním světě World of Warcraft. Nepublikovaná diplomová práce. Masarykova univerzita.

Hewstone (1989). Causal attribution: from cognitive processes to collective beliefs. Oxford: Blackwell.

Higgins, E.T., King, G. A., & Mavin,G. H. (1982). Individual construct accessibility and subjective impressions and recall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 35–47.

Hilton, D. J. (1990). Conversational processes and causal explanation. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 1, 65–81.

Horgan, T. & Woodward, J. (1999). Folk psychology is here to stay. In W. G. Lycan (Ed.). Mind and Cognition (271–286). Malden: Blackwell.

Horváth, J. (2009). Rasové předsudky u policistů. Nepublikovaná diplomová práce. Masarykova univerzita.

Hurlburt, R. T. & Schwitzgebel, E. (2007). Describing inner experience? Proponent meets skeptic. Cambridge: A Bradford Book. The MIT Press.

Jack, A. & Roepstorff, A. (Eds.). (2003). Trusting the subject? The use of introspective evidence in cognitive science. Vol. 1. Thorverton: Imprint Academic.

Jack, A. & Roepstorff, A. (Eds.). (2004). Trusting the subject? The use of introspective evidence in cognitive science. Vol. 2. Thorverton: Imprint Academic.

James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology [online]. [cit. 10–03–2008]. Dostupné z WWW: <http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/…es/prin7.htm>

Kahneman, D., Slovic, P. & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Kratochvíl, S. (1999). Experimentální hypnóza (2. přeprac. vyd.). Praha: Academia.

Laming, D. R. J. (2004). Understanding human motivation: what makes people tick? Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

Darley, J. M. & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of resposiblity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8(4), 377–383.

Lyons, W. (1986). The Disappearance of Introspection. Cambridge: A Bradford Book. The MIT Press.

Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral Study of obedience. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(4), 371–378.

Myers, D. G. (2008). Determined and free. In J. Baer, J. C. Kaufman & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Are we free. Psychology and free will, 32–43. New York: Oxford University Press.

Nisbett, R. E. & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know. Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review, 84 (3), s. 231–259.

Simon, H. (1957). Models of man: Social and rational. New York: Wiley.

Shariff, A. F., Schooler, J. & Vohs, K. D. (2008). The hazards of claiming to have solved the hard problem of free will. In J. Baer, J. C. Kaufman & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Are we free. Psychology and free will (181–204). New York: Oxford University Press.

Payne, B. K., Jacoby, L. L., & Lambert, A. J. (2005). Attitudes as accessibility bias: dissociating automatic and controlled components. In R. Hassin, J. Bargh, J. & Uleman (Eds.), The new unconscious (393–420). New York: Oxford University Press.

Pinker, S. (2008). The fear of determinism. In J. Baer, J. C. Kaufman & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Are we free. Psychology and free will (311–324). New York: Oxford University Press.

Pronin, E. & Kugler, M. B. (2007). Valuing thoughts, ignoring behavior: The introspection illusion as a source of the bias blind spot. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 565–578.

Pronin, E., Kruger, J., Savitsky, K. & Ross, L. (2001). You don't know me, but I know you: asymmetric assessment of insight into self and other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 639–656.

Roediger, H. R., Goode, M. K. & Zaromb, F. M. (2008). Free will and the control of action. In J. Baer, J. C. Kaufman & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Are We Free. Psychology and Free Will (205–225). New York: Oxford University Press.

Rosenthal, R. (1991). Teacher expectancy effects: A brief update 25 years after the Pygmalion experiment. Journal of Research in Education, 1, 3–12.

Sacks, O. (1993). Muž, který si pletl manželku s kloboukem a jiné klinické povídky (orig. The man who mistook his wife for a hat and other clinical tales, 1983; přel. A. Čechová). Praha: Mladá fronta.

Shah, K. A. & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). Heuristics made easy: An effort-reduction framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 2, 207–222.

The Economist (2008). In Jak mozek nakupuje. Respekt, 52, 1, 34–36.

Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1978). Salience, attention, and attribution: Top of the head phenomena. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 11 (249–288). New York: Academic Press.

Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (3–22). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J. & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey response. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Varela, F. & Shear, J. (Eds.). (2002, second reprint, orig. 1999). From view from within. First-person approaches to the study of consciousness. Thoverton: Imprint Academic.

Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge: A Bradford Book. The MIT Press.

Wegner, D. M. (2005). Who is the controller of controlled processes? In R. R. Hassin, J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious (19–36). New York: Oxford University Press.

White, P. A. (1992). Causal powers, causal questions, and the place of regularity information in causal attribution. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 161–188.

Willis, G. B. (2005). Cognitive interviewing. A tool for improving questionnaire design. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves. Discovering the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Wilson, T. D. (2003). Knowing when to ask. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10, 131–140.

Wilson, T. D. & Dunn, E. W. (2004). Self-knowledge: its limits, value, and potential for improvement. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 493–518.

Zvolský, P. & Raboch, J. (Eds.). (2001). Psychiatrie. Praha: Galén.

Show all Hide